Noticing Confusion

Entry Question: How do we notice when we are confused about some topic?


Surprise

If you are surprised by something, it means that you made a prediction and it was wrong. You did constrain your anticipation - you said that X would happen, and Y would not happen. But you were wrong - Y happened anyway. That’s cool. You can update on this evidence. You can throw out the belief that predicted Y won’t happen (cos this is very strong evidence against it).

However, if you weren’t surprised by wrong answers, it means that you haven’t been making predictions at all.

Making predictions implies that you will either be reassured or be surprised by the outcome. You either got it right or you got it wrong.

But when you don’t make predictions, when your beliefs don’t constrain anticipation, you will never be surprised by any outcome.

Experiment time!

Ok. How do we find out whether you are confused about something?

You say you have a correct model about something.

Well, what could the reality be? You could actually have a correct model. You could have a wrong model. Or, you could be confused.

It’s my job as a scientist to find out the truth here.

What are the hypotheses?

Assume X is an event with two outcomes, A and B.

[For now consider the simple case where you’re either always right (H2) or always wrong (H3). We will deal with more complex hypotheses later.]

How do we design hypothesis tests to distinguish between these them?

We look at the places where they make differing predictions.

We know that if you’re confused, you won’t be surprised by anything.

Hmmm… Could they differ in the level of surprise we would have? Let’s see.

Variables

Assumption: The actual outcome of X is A.

There is one independent variable - the input:

The dependent variable - the output / prediction:

We want to see if the hypotheses give different predictions for the same input.

Cases

A has happened.

Observations

Yes! We have our differing predictions!

  1. Surprised at the actual outcome: you have the wrong model.

  2. Surprised at the false outcome: you have the correct model.

  3. Not surprised at either outcome: you, sir, are confused!

When you’re not confused, whether you’re right or wrong, you have the possibility of surprise. That’s all we need.


More complex scenarios

Let’s look at a more realistic scenario. In the real world, you are not always wrong. Sometimes, you are right; sometimes, you are wrong. i.e., you may understand some parts of the topic well, and misunderstand the other parts.

There’s an event X, with possible outcomes A, B, C, and D.

Actual outcome: A.

Hypotheses:

Again, the input variable is what I tell you (“A happened”, …) and the output variable is your surprise.

Cases

This time, let’s look at the likelihood of not being surprised in each case.

Note: Likelihood of surprise = 1 - Likelihood of no surprise

I tell you A happened

i.e., I tell you the actual outcome

Likelihood ratio for no surprise = 100 : 100 : 25 : 0

If you’re not surprised by the correct answer, you certainly don’t have a completely wrong model (H4).

But it is pretty strong evidence that you don’t have a partially wrong model (H3).

Likelihood ratio for surprise = 0 : 0 : 75 : 100

So, if you’re surprised by the correct answer, your model is definitely wrong (partially or fully).

I tell you, at random, that B happened

i.e., I tell you a false outcome

Likelihood ratio for no surprise = 100 : 0 : 33 : 33

If you’re not surprised by a wrong answer, you certainly do not have a correct model (H2).

Also, if you’re not surprised by a random wrong answer, then it becomes more and more likely that you are actually Confused (H1), not just wrong.

Likelihood ratio for surprise = 0 : 100 : 66 : 66

If you’re surprised by a wrong answer, you are not confused. Period. You could have a correct model, or a partially wrong model, or a fully wrong model. But you’re not confused.

Observations

We have plenty of differing predictions here. Look at the ’0’s in the Likelihood ratios.

  1. Not surprised by a false answer: you don’t have a correct model.

    You need to be surprised by every single false answer I give you.

  2. Not surprised by a Random False Answer: strong evidence that you are confused.

    The chances of your model coming up with the exact same answer as my random answer are very slim.

  3. Surprised at some answer (whether it was right or wrong): strong evidence that you are not confused

    Confused people don’t usually get surprised.

  4. Surprised by a correct answer: your model is definitely wrong.

    But, on the bright side, you are definitely not confused.

  5. Not surprised by a correct answer: you don’t have a completely wrong model.

    Congrats. Your model is not completely useless.

Your Strength as a Rationalist

Here comes the bang!

If somebody claims that they know a topic well, then they should be able to make correct predictions, right?

Now, as per (A), I lie to them about the result. I give them some Random False Answer.

If they are surprised, it means they are not confused! Good. They should be surprised. I gave them the wrong result.

If they don’t get surprised, hey! They don’t have a correct model! They accepted a wrong answer! That is just stupid (like thinking that the lighter ball will reach the ground first. Or that the heavier ball will :P)

Are they confused or do they have a wrong model? As per (B), I give them more of such Random False Answers. If they don’t get surprised often, it becomes extremely likely that they are confused.

False results should surprise you if you know the subject well.

And so it is said:

Your strength as a rationalist is your ability to be more confused by fiction than by reality. If you are equally good at explaining any outcome, you have zero knowledge.

Your Strength as a Rationalist, Eliezer Yudkowsky

“I haven’t understood X fully”

That’s why I am not able to answer questions correctly. All I need to do is sit and read some more books for a while. Then, I will beat your sorry little ass easily.”

Oh, really? Is that so?

Does he have a lack of understanding about X (wrong model) or is he actually plain confused?

Easy to find out! Just use (B) and (D).

Give him a bunch of Random False Answers, maybe by casually dropping some “facts” in conversation. If he is not surprised, it means he is confused about the topic, not wrong. He doesn’t have any model in his head. “Sitting” and “reading books” is not gonna fix his confusion.

On the other hand, if you give some Random False Answers or a few correct answers, and he is surprised, then he just has a wrong model. He can correct it by reading up on the topic.

The same test can be used for anybody who says that they don’t quite remember what they have studied about a subject. Deploy the Random False Answers test. They may actually be confused, not forgetful.

Ditto for what people say about esoteric subjects (like Quantum Mechanics or Evolution). Check whether they (or you) are surprised by Random False Answers (“Cockroaches started evolving 16 billion years ago. That’s why they can survive even nuclear attacks” - wrong on so many different levels). Check whether they (or you) are surprised by correct answers. If you have a wrong model, you should be surprised at least some times. If you’re nodding your head all along the way, you were, are, and will remain confused.

What’s the big difference?

We wanted to identify when you say you know a topic but are actually confused. Similarly, we wanted to identify when you say you have a wrong understanding about a topic but are actually confused (not just wrong).

But, whether you think you’re right but are confused, or whether you think you’re wrong but are actually confused, or whether you’re just plain wrong but not confused, the outcome is the same: you get poor results.

Why do we care?

We care because the remedies are different.

When you’re wrong but not confused, you can improve your predictions by getting more information. You can update on the evidence. The road to recovery is straightforward.

But when you’re confused, whether you think you’re right or wrong, the road ahead is completely different. More information will not solve your problem. Consulting with others won’t help. Neither will banging your head against the problem.

When you’re confused, you won’t be surprised by fake answers. When you’re confused, you won’t be surprised by right answers that you hadn’t guessed (and may never have guessed, like the two balls reaching the ground at the same time). This means that you cannot learn from experience. You can’t improve your performance over time.


This is just one way of Noticing Confusion. When you’re Confused, you don’t get surprised by anything.

But that is not the only way your Confusion exhibits itself.

(to be continued)

PS

Notes

Created: November 18, 2014
Last modified: August 6, 2015
Status: finished
Tags: confusion, anticipation constraint

comments powered by Disqus